These findings also suggest that reversal can be explained in an entirely different way. When one does not know that a figure can be seen in more than one way, one tends to organize it on the basis of certain principles—–for instance, in favor of the central region of Rubin’s vase and faces as figure rather than ground or, in the case of the Necker cube, in favor of the depth organization in which the cube appears to be horizontal rather than tipped on an edge. These preferences were in fact manifested in our experiment. Of course, if no such lawful factors are operating, the initial organization might be based on chance factors, such as where in the figure the observer happens to be fixating or attending. Once that organization occurs, the naive observer may stick with it, there being no particular reason to alter it. After a long period of inspection, which is only likely to occur in experiments, the observer may become bored (satiated, in the psychological meaning of the word). Or the observer may begin to wonder why he or she is supposed to continue looking at it and begin to look for alternatives, which of course would change the equation. Or the observer’s attention may wander, thus interfering with the maintenance of the initial organization. Obviously, naive observers sometimes do achieve a reversal, or reversible figures would never have been discovered.